Borderline Personality Features in Childhood: The Role of Subtype, Developmental Timing and Chronicity of Child Maltreatment (2024)

As a library, NLM provides access to scientific literature. Inclusion in an NLM database does not imply endorsem*nt of, or agreement with, the contents by NLM or the National Institutes of Health.
Learn more: PMC Disclaimer | PMC Copyright Notice

Borderline Personality Features in Childhood: The Role of Subtype, Developmental Timing and Chronicity of Child Maltreatment (1)

About Author manuscriptsSubmit a manuscriptHHS Public Access; Author Manuscript; Accepted for publication in peer reviewed journal;

Dev Psychopathol. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2015 Aug 1.

Published in final edited form as:

PMCID: PMC4141853

NIHMSID: NIHMS616205

PMID: 25047300

Author information Copyright and License information PMC Disclaimer

The publisher's final edited version of this article is available at Dev Psychopathol

Abstract

Child maltreatment has been established as a risk factor for borderline personality disorder (BPD), yet few studies consider how maltreatment influences the development of BPD features through childhood and adolescence. Subtype, developmental timing and chronicity of child maltreatment were examined as factors in the development of borderline personality features in childhood. Children (M age = 11.30, SD = 0.94), including 314 maltreated and 285 nonmaltreated children from comparable low socioeconomic backgrounds, provided self-reports of developmentally salient borderline personality traits. Maltreated children had higher overall borderline feature scores, higher scores on each individual subscale and were more likely to be identified as at high risk for development of BPD through raised scores on all 4 subscales. Chronicity of maltreatment predicted higher overall borderline feature scores and patterns of onset and recency of maltreatment significantly predicted whether a participant would meet criteria for the high-risk group. Implications of findings and recommendations for intervention are discussed.

Introduction

In recent years, a growing body of literature on personalit and child development has made it increasingly clear that 1) personality pathology is not exclusive to adulthood and 2) some personality disorders diagnosed in adulthood are likely to have developmental pathways which begin in childhood (Bemporad, Smith, Hansen, & Cicchetti, 1992, ; Cicchetti & Olson, 1990; DeClercq, De Fruyt, van Leeuen, & Mervielde, 2006; ; ; Shiner, 2009). Although the pathogenesis of personality disorder has been studied extensively with respect to the role of adverse childhood experiences (; Zanarini, 2000), no single risk factor has received as much focus as child maltreatment. Research has demonstrated that personality disorders are more prevalent among individuals with a history of abuse in childhood (; Johnson et al., 2000; ; Joyce et al., 2003). Further, recent longitudinal studies have shown that childhood abuse and neglect are predictive of an increased risk for personality pathology in adulthood (Johnson et al., 2006; ; ). One longitudinal study showed that having a documented child abuse experience made an individual over 4 times more likely to be diagnosed with a personality disorder in adulthood, even after controlling for parental education, age and parental psychiatric history (Johnson et al.,1999).

Borderline personality disorder (BPD) in particular has received strong attention as a possible outcome of child maltreatment. BPD is a serious form of psychopathology characterized by a complex constellation of social, cognitive, emotional and behavioral dysregulation, most notably features of affective instability and dysregulation, impulsivity, dysfunctional interpersonal relationships and identity problems (American Psychiatric Association, 2000). In addition to its concerning and extensive symptomatology, the disorder also is notably prevalent in severely impaired populations requiring intensive or inpatient psychiatric care. BPD is the most common Axis II disorder seen in inpatient psychiatric settings; Although the disorder is estimated at rates of 1-6% in the general adult population, it is thought to be present in 10-15% of clinical populations (Lenzenweger, 2008; Skodol et. al 2002; ; ; ; Widiger & Weisman, 1991). BPD sufferers often require extensive mental health care. BPD criteria are met in 19% of psychiatric hospitalizations and the rate of suicide in individuals with the disorder is close to 50 times the rate of the population (American Psychiatric Association, 2000; ).

Research indicates that maltreatment is a serious risk factor for the development of BPD. Up to 71% of BPD patients report severe abuse in childhood (; ; ) and history of childhood abuse has been shown to predict borderline symptoms independent of family environment and parental psychopathology (Bradley, Jeneai & Westen, 2006). A review of relevant literature by found support for childhood trauma as a causative factor in the development of BPD. This is a crucial consideration when examined in concert with current longitudinal research supporting personality as an outcome that develops over time in response to a dynamic and transactional process between environmental and genetic influences (; Belsky et al. 2012; ; ; Shiner, 2009).

If personality is a dynamic system developing over time, then prodromal symptoms of personality pathology should become apparent in developmentally relevant ways. In fact, content analysis of DSM criteria by found five childhood indicators of BPD: hostile or paranoid worldview; impulsivity; intense, unstable or inappropriate emotion; excessively close relationships; and lack of sense of self. Indeed, many noted sequelae of child maltreatment form a pattern of dysfunction in childhood that is strikingly similar to both these criteria and BPD in adulthood. Child victims of maltreatment have shown disruptions in peer, familial and romantic relations, impulsivity and attentional difficulties, dysregulation of affect, and negative or disrupted representations of self (; ; ; ; Toth et al., 2000). Additionally, maltreated children are at higher risk for youth suicidal ideation or behavior (). This is especially notable as up to 30% of adults with BPD report the onset of self-injurious behavior prior to the age of 12 (Zanarini et al., 2006).

The similarities between characteristics of BPD in adulthood and the featural pattern of deficits associated with maltreatment suggest a possible pathway by which BPD develops. However, major limitations present in the current literature circ*mscribe conclusions that may be drawn by researchers in this arena.

First, current studies relating maltreatment to BPD have traditionally relied on retrospective reports of maltreatment by adults identified as meeting criteria for BPD. Such studies are limited by retrospective identification of maltreatment experiences, as any significant relationships found may be influenced by participant response bias or recall bias (Johnson et al., 2005; ). As note in their discussion of factors that influence recall, an individual's current state, including psychopathological symptoms has been shown to influence recall ability (for a review of literature on state-dependent recall, see Blaney, 1986). Moreover, as noted by Shiner (2009), this issue of recall bias is exceptionally salient in the realm of personality pathology due to the distortion of perception inherent within the class of disorders. Distorted interpersonal perceptions are fundamental to the nature of BPD symptoms in particular.

Second, although it is clear that maltreatment is a serious risk factor for later personality pathology and BPD specifically, previous studies have generally focused on adult personality outcomes in their investigations (Paris, 2005). This is unfortunate, as information on personality development and outcomes through childhood and adolescence may help to illuminate the specific functional mechanisms by which maltreatment impacts later personality. In addition, examination of childhood personality processes may also elucidate prodromal signs of personality pathology, better allowing researchers to develop and implement early interventions. To our knowledge, no studies to date have examined self-report of developmentally salient BPD symptoms in maltreated children.

Finally, when considering maltreatment as a risk factor in the development of personality pathology, researchers too often ignore the myriad differences in individual maltreatment experiences. Laudably, researchers have increasingly moved away from a “presence versus absence” dichotomy of maltreatment experience when considering abuse or neglect in analyses. However, many maltreatment parameters are frequently ignored such as the chronicity and timing of maltreatment. When included in analyses, information on the developmental timing, onset and recency of maltreatment can produce a more nuanced understanding of the impact of maltreatment on individual neurobiological, psychological, emotional and behavioral outcomes (; ; ). Indeed, studies by and Thornberry, Ireland and Smith (2001) found that the developmental period in which maltreatment first occurred differentially impacted outcomes on a host of maladaptive factors, including internalizing and externalizing symptomatology, delinquency, teen pregnancy, and alcohol and drug use. A recent examination by Jaffee and Maikovich-Fong (2011) replicated facets of these findings; maltreatment occurring in multiple developmental periods compared to a single developmental period was similarly found to be related to increased internalizing and externalizing problems.. However, these differences became non-significant once family and environmental characteristics such as caregiver depression, arrest history, educational qualifications, and neighborhood danger were controlled for in analysis. Additionally, an investigation by Jonson-Reid, Kohl and Drake (2012) found that chronicity was a strong predictor of negative child and adult outcomes such as substance use, violent delinquency, suicide attempts and mental health treatment. To date, work focused on the relation between maltreatment and the development of BPD has not considered chronicity and developmental timing of maltreatment.

The goal of the present investigation is to examine the impact of maltreatment subtype, timing and chronicity in the development of four major categories of developmentally salient borderline features: affective instability, identity problems, negative relationships, and self-harm.

Hypotheses

  1. Maltreated children, compared to nonmaltreated children, will display higher overall levels of borderline features.

  2. Maltreated children will have significantly higher scores on 4 the major borderline feature scales: affective instability (AI), identity problems (IP), negative relationships (NR), and self-harm (SH).

  3. Maltreatment subtype, including physical abuse (PA), sexual abuse (SA), emotional maltreatment (EM) and physical neglect (PN) will impact the degree of borderline features present.

  4. The maltreated group will be more likely to have high scores on all 4 borderline feature subscales.

  5. Maltreatment chronicity, onset and recency will predict overall borderline feature scores.

  6. Maltreatment chronicity, onset and recency will predict rates of individuals with elevated scores on all 4 subscales.

Method

Participants

The current study was focused on 10- to 12-year-olds. Participants included 599 children (M age =11.30, SD = 0.94) who attended a summer camp research program designed for low-income school-aged children. The sample was comprised of children who had been maltreated (n = 314) and comparison children who had not been maltreated (n = 285). Children were residing with their biological mothers and had not attended any previous camp research program. Over 90% of the families of both the maltreated and the nonmaltreated children had histories of receiving public assistance. Demographic characteristics are presented in Table 1. No significant differences were found between the maltreated group and the comparison group, in terms of age, gender, or race/ethnicity.

Table 1

Demographic Characteristics

VariableMaltreated group (n=314)Nonmaltreated group (n=314)
M(SD) or %M(SD) or %
Age11.30 (0.96)11.30 (0.92)
Gender (% male)49.0%49.5%
Race/Ethnicity
 African American70.0%66.0%
 Latino14.022.8
 Caucasian11.88.1
 Other4.13.1

Note. Differences between groups are ns.

Parents of all children provided informed consent for participation in addition to providing consent for examination of Department of Human Services (DHS) records regarding the family history. The maltreated group was recruited based on information contained in DHS records for children identified by the county as having experienced at least one form of child abuse and/or neglect. The sample was representative of DHS service recipient families within the county. To recruit families, a liaison from DHS contacted families with a child who had been maltreated to inform them of their eligibility for the study. The study was explained to caregivers, and if parents were interested then they signed a release to have their names provided to team members for recruitment. Participation was voluntary and families could decline to participate without any negative consequences from DHS. If a family elected to participate, after providing consent a comprehensive search of DHS records was completed to independently code for maltreatment information. Maltreatment was identified and categorized using operational criteria specified in the Maltreatment Classification System (MCS: ). The coding process used in the analysis of DHS records is described in detail in Barnett et al. (1993). Mothers also were interviewed by research staff using the Maternal Maltreatment Classification Interview to (MMCI: ) to determine if additional maltreatment experiences may have occurred that were not known to DHS.

Maltreated children in this sample were predominantly from low socioeconomic status families, as is consistent with the national demographic characteristics of maltreating families (Fourth National Incidence Study of Child Abuse and Neglect [NIS-4]; Sedlack et al., 2010). Consequently, demographically comparable nonmaltreated children were recruited from families receiving Temporary Assistance for Needy Families (TANF). A DHS liaison contacted qualifying nonmaltreating families to explain the study and inform them of their eligibility. If interested, parents signed a release for their names to be given to the project for recruitment. DHS record searches also were completed for nonmaltreating families to verify the absence of any record of child maltreatment. Once recruited, trained research assistants interviewed mothers of children in the nonmaltreatment group using the MMCI to confirm a lack of DHS involvement pertaining to prior maltreatment experiences. Only children without any documented or maternally reported history of abuse or neglect were retained in the nonmaltreated group. Record searches also were conducted in the year following camp attendance to ensure that all available information had been obtained.

Classification of the maltreated group based on the experiences of various maltreatment subtypes was accomplished via the MCS (Barnett et al., 1993). The MCS is a reliable and valid tool for classifying subtype(s) of maltreatment (; English et al., 2005; ) that utilizes DHS records detailing investigations and findings involving maltreatment in identified families over time. The MCS codes all available information on an individual based on DHS data and makes independent determinations of maltreatment experiences rather than relying on official designations or case dispositions. Operational criteria are used to designate all subtypes of maltreatment that children have experienced (i.e., neglect, emotional maltreatment, physical abuse, sexual abuse) and the severity of each subtype experienced.

The MCS also uses documented DHS records to determine when in the course of development maltreatment occurred. Maltreatment events were coded as occurring during one of five developmental periods, including Infancy (0 to 18 months), Toddlerhood (19-35 months), Preschool (36-59 months), Early School Age (ages 5 to 7), and Later School Age (ages 8 to 12). The number of developmental periods where maltreatment was known to have occurred was then totaled to produce an index of chronicity of maltreatment. The developmental period of onset and the recency of maltreatment also were determined. This information was used to identify 3 groups among maltreated children: 1) Early Onset, Not Recent (n = 107), 2) Early Onset, Recent (n = 89) and 3) Later Onset, Recent (n = 118). The Early Onset, Not Recent group was defined as individuals who had maltreatment onset in the Infancy, Toddler or Preschool period and most recent maltreatment experience in the Infancy, Toddler or Preschool period. The Early Onset, Recent group was defined at individuals who had maltreatment onset in the Infancy, Toddler or Preschool period and most recent maltreatment experience in the Early School Age or Later School Age period. Later Onset, Recent was defined at individuals who had maltreatment onset in the Early School Age or Later School Age period and most recent maltreatment experience in the Early School Age or Later School Age period.

Coding of the DHS records was conducted by trained research assistants, doctoral students, and clinical psychologists. Coders were required to meet acceptable reliability with criterion standards before coding actual records for the study. Reliability was established for the presence versus absence of each maltreatment subtype (κs = .90 to 1.00).

In terms of the subtypes of maltreatment, neglect involves failure to provide for the child's basic physical needs for adequate food, clothing, shelter, and medical treatment. Emotional maltreatment involves extreme thwarting of children's basic emotional needs for psychological safety and security, acceptance and self-esteem, and age-appropriate autonomy. Physical abuse involves the non-accidental infliction of physical injury on the child (e.g., bruises, welts, burns, choking, broken bones). Finally, sexual abuse involves attempted or actual sexual contact between the child and a family member or person caring for the child for purposes of that person's sexual satisfaction or financial benefit.

All children in the maltreatment group had a history of abuse and/or neglect as documented by DHS records. Of the maltreated group, 79.9% had experienced physical neglect, 53% had experienced emotional maltreatment, 28.9% had experienced a form of physical abuse, and 8.6% experienced a form of sexual abuse. The majority of children experienced multiple subtypes of maltreatment, with 56.2% of the maltreated children having experienced two or more subtypes of maltreatment. In this sample, all 15 possible combinations of maltreatment subtypes were represented. This comorbidity of maltreatment subtypes is typical in previous literature examining developmental sequelae of maltreatment (Manly, 2005)

Participants were classified as one of the four primary subtypes of maltreatment based on a hierarchical classification system (e.g., ; Rogosch & Cicchetti 2005) that takes into consideration the degree to which the subtype of maltreatment violates cultural standards and the relative frequency of the maltreatment form. Based on this classification system, any child who experienced sexual abuse was categorized as sexually abused (n = 27). Any child who had been physically abused but not sexually abused was classified as physically abused (n = 78). A child who experienced neglect but had not experienced physical or sexual abuse was classified as physically neglected (n = 194), and children who had experienced emotional maltreatment absent of all other forms of maltreatment were classified as emotionally maltreated (n = 15).

Procedure

Children attended a week-long day camp from 9:00am to 4:00pm and participated in both traditional camp activities and research assessments (see , for detailed descriptions of camp procedures). Children were transported by bus to the camp each day, with travel time averaging 45 min. At the camp, children were assigned to groups of eight (four maltreatment, four comparison) same-age and same-sex peers. Each group was led by three trained camp counselors who were unaware of the maltreatment status of children and the hypotheses of the study. In addition to recreational activities, after providing assent, children participated in various research assessments. Trained research assistants conducted these individual research sessions in which questionnaires and other research measures were administered. This paper focuses on maltreatment parameters as related to responses on the Borderline Personality Features Scale for Children (BPFS-C) ().

Measures

Borderline Personality Features Scale for Children (BPFS-C)

Borderline traits were measured using the BPFS-C (), a self-report questionnaire used to measure borderline personality features in youth. The scale is based off of the Personality Assessment Inventory (PAI) (Morey, 1991), a measure used to assess borderline personality pathology among adults. The BPFS-C has demonstrated reliability and validity and was developed in consultation with the author of the PAI. The four subscales of the BPFS-C are the same as the subscales of the PAI: affective instability, identity problems, negative relationships and self-harm. However, Crick and colleagues (2005) have adapted the items so they reflect age- appropriate indicators (e.g., middle childhood and beyond) of borderline personality pathology. For example, affective instability is measured with questions such as, “My feelings are very strong. For instance, when I get mad, I get really really mad. When I get happy, I get really really happy” and “I go back and forth between different feelings, like being mad or sad or happy.” Children report on identity problems through questions such as, “I feel that something important is missing about me, but I don't know what it is” and “I change my mind almost every day about what I should do when I grow up.” Examples of negative relationship subscale questions include, “I want to let some people know how much they've hurt me” and “I've picked friends who have treated me badly,” while self-harm questions include, “When I get upset, I do things that aren't good for me” and “I get into trouble because I do things without thinking.” Children rate each statement on a Likert scale based on how true the statement is of themselves, from 1 (not at all true) to 5 (always true).

Each of the four subscales of the BPFS-C have 6 items for a total of 24 items. The Likert scores for these items are summed (some items are reverse-scored) to generate a composite borderline personality features score, with higher scores indicative of higher levels of borderline traits. Crick et al. (2005) reports that the measure has established construct validity. In the current investigation, internal consistency for the measure was α = .83.

Results

Borderline features in maltreated and nonmaltreated groups

Overall borderline feature scores on the BPFS-C were compared in maltreated and nonmaltreated groups. An analysis of variance (ANOVA) revealed that maltreated children reported significantly higher levels of borderline features than nonmaltreated children, F(1, 590) = 28.83, p < .001, η2 = .047. Additional ANOVAs were performed to determine whether significant differences were present between maltreated and comparison groups on each of the four subscales. On each subscale, maltreated children had significantly higher scores than nonmaltreated children. Findings for each subscale are reported in Table 2.

Table 2

Borderline features in maltreated and non-maltreated groups

VariableMaltreatedComparison.dfFη2p
M (SD)M (SD)
BPFS-C sum score68.68 (15.04)62.43 (13.06)128.82.047.000
Subscales
 Affective Instability20.12 (4.34)18.55 (4.14)120.37.033.000
 Identity Problems16.61 (5.45)15.01 (4.78)114.31.023.000
 Negative Relationships15.29 (4.29)13.92 (3.66)117.37.028.000
 Self Harm16.62 (4.81)14.98 (4.03)120.26.033.000

BPFS-C sum score by maltreatment subtype

Total borderline features scores were compared by individual maltreatment subtype (EM, PN, PA, SA, Comparison). The findings are summarized in Table 3. Results showed a main effect of individual subtype on overall borderline features, F(4, 587) = 7.70, p < .001, η2 = .050. Pairwise comparisons indicated that physically neglected children had significantly higher borderline features scores than the nonmaltreated comparison group (mean difference = 6.25, SE = 1.32, p < .001). Physically abused children also had significantly higher borderline feature scores than the comparison group, mean difference = 7.12, SE = 1.81, p = .001). Emotionally maltreated and sexually abused children did not differ from nonmaltreated children with respect to overall borderline features. Maltreatment subtypes did not significantly differ among one another in overall borderline features.

Table 3

Borderline features by maltreatment subtype

VariableEMPNPASAComp.Fpη2Post Hoc
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
M
(SD)
BPFS-C Sum Score70.07
(15.53)
68.69
(15.35)
69.56
(14.51)
65.19
(14.37)
62.43
(13.05)
7.70.000.050Comp < PN***, PA***
BPFS-C subscales
 Affective Instability20.87
(4.00)
20.25
(4.53)
20.01
(3.89)
19.11
(4.48)
18.55
(4.14)
5.64.000.037Comp < PN***
 Identity Problems15.40
(5.44)
16.74
(5.65)
16.74
(5.17)
5.89
(4.82)
15.01
(4.78)
3.95.004.026Comp < PN**
 Negative Relationships15.86
(5.41)
15.22
(4.33)
15.56
(4.04)
14.70
(4.25)
13.92
(3.66)
4.65.001.030Comp < PN**, PA*
 Self Harm17.93
(4.53)
16.45
(4.80)
17.14
(4.70)
15.61
(5.28)
14.97
(4.02)
6.07.000.040Comp < PN**, PA***

Note. EM = Emotional Maltreatment, PN = Physical Neglect, PA = Physical Abuse, SA = Sexual Abuse, Comp. = Comparison;.

*p < .05.

***p < .001.

BPFS-C subscales by maltreatment subtype

Scores on each of the four BPFS-C subscales were also compared by individual maltreatment subtype (EM, PN, PA, SA, Comparison). Results showed a main effect of individual subtype on all four scales: affective instability, F(4, 587) = 4.64, p < .001, η2 = .037, identity problems, F(4, 587) = 3.95, p < .01, η2 = .026, negative relationships, F(4, 587) = 4.65, p < .01, η2 = .030, and self harm F(4, 587) = 6.07, p < .01, η2 = .047. Pairwise comparisons indicated that physically neglected children had significantly higher scores than nonmaltreated children on all four subscales: affective instability (mean difference = 1.70, SE = .40, p < .001), identity problems (mean difference = 1.73, SE = .48, p = .003), negative relationships, (mean difference = 1.30, SE = .37, p = .005), and self harm (mean difference = 1.47, SE = .41, p = .001). Relative to nonmaltreated children, physically abused children also endorsed more related to negative relationships (mean difference = 1.63, SE = .52, p = .01) and self harm (mean difference = 2.16, SE = .57, p < .001). Emotionally maltreated children and sexually abused children did not significantly differ from nonmaltreated children on any of the four subscales. Maltreatment subtypes did not significantly differ among one another with regard to the subscales.

Borderline features by number of maltreatment subtypes

In order to determine the impact of exposure to multiple types of maltreatment experiences on development of borderline features, overall borderline features were compared in children who had experienced 1 or 2 subtypes of maltreatment to those who had experienced 3 or 4 subtypes. Differences were found between groups in overall borderline features, F(2, 589) = 14.49, p < .001, η2 = .047. Tukey post-hoc analyses showed significant differences in mean scores between the comparison group (M = 62.43, SD = 13.06) and both 1-2 subtypes (M = 68.82, SD = 15.14), p > .001 and 3-4 subtypes (M = 67.88, SD = 14.63) p < .05. No significant differences were found between 1-2 subtypes and 3-4 subtypes.

Borderline features by number of developmental periods maltreated

Linear regression analysis was used to test if the number of developmental periods in which maltreatment occurred, i.e., chronicity, significantly predicted participants' borderline personality feature scores. The results of the regression indicated the number of developmental periods with maltreatment explained 4.4% of the variance, R2=.044, F(1,590)=28.23, p<.001. It was found that greater chronicity significantly predicted higher borderline features, B=2.892, S.E.=.544, p<.001.

Borderline features by maltreatment onset and recency

An ANOVA was used to examine overall borderline feature scores as a function of patterns of onset and recency of maltreatment. Differences were found in overall borderline features by onset and recency group, F(3, 588) = 11.21, p < .001, η2 = .057. Tukey post hoc analyses revealed significant differences in mean scores between the nonmaltreated group (M = 62.43, SD = 13.06) and the Early Onset, Not Recent group (M = 67.49, SD = 15.95), p < .01, the Early Onset, Recent group (M = 71.42, SD = 15.91), p < .001, and the Later Onset, Recent group (M = 67.67, SD = 13.31), p < .01. No significant differences were found in mean borderline feature scores between the different onset and recency categories.

Borderline Features by Gender

Borderline feature scores were compared by sex to investigate the role of gender. Males and females did not differ in overall BPFS-C scores by gender, F(1,590)= 0.315, ns. Rates of high scores on the BPFS-C sum also did not differ by gender, with 17.3% of males and 19.5% of females scoring 1 SD or more over the mean, χ2(1, N = 592) = 0.53, ns.

High-risk group

As diagnosis of BPD requires noted impairment 5 of the 9 BPD criteria listed in the DSM-IV, it is important to examine children who have high scores on most or all of the BPFS-C borderline feature subscales, as these children may be at especially high risk for development of BPD later in life. We defined this high-risk group as individuals with elevated scores on all BPFS-C subscales, with three subscales more than 1 SD above the mean.In the full sample, 39 children met criteria for this high-risk group, including 4.3% of nonmaltreated children (comprising 30.8% of the high-risk group) and 8.6% of maltreated children (69.2% of the high risk group). This rate significantly differed, χ2(1, N = 599) = 4.73, p <.05. The high-risk group was examined by maltreatment subtype as well. Relative to 4.3% of nonmaltreated children, 13.3% of the EM group, 7.7% of the PN group, 11.5% of the PA group, and 3.7% of the SA group met inclusion criteria for the high risk group. Relative to the comparison group, rates of inclusion in the high risk group significantly differed in the PA group, χ2(1, N = 363) = 6.03, p <.05, but did not significantly differ in the EM, PN and SA groups, respectively EM χ2(1, N = 300) = 2.66, ns, PN χ2(1, N = 479) = 2.69, ns, and SA χ2(1, N = 312) = 0.16, ns,. Rate of inclusion in the high-risk group was also examined by number of maltreatment subtypes (1-2 versus 3-4). Of the 1-2 subtypes group, 8.9% met criteria for the high risk group, whereas in the 3-4 subtypes group, 8.0% met criteria for inclusion. This rate significantly differed between the comparison group and the 1-2 subtypes group, χ2(1, N = 565) = 5.14, p <.05, but did not significantly differ between the comparison group and the 3-4 subtypes group.

Inclusion in high-risk group by number of developmental periods maltreated

Logistic regression analysis was used to test if the number of developmental periods in which maltreatment occurred significantly predicted whether a participant would meet criteria for the high-risk group. The overall model was not significant, χ2=3.195, df=1, p=.074. The number of developmental periods maltreated also was not significant, B=.250, S.E.=.134, p=.062, OR = 1.284.

Inclusion in high-risk group by maltreatment onset and recency

Logistic regression analysis was used to test if patterns of onset and recency of maltreatment significantly predicted whether a participant would meet criteria for the high-risk group. The overall model was significant, χ2=10.116, df=3, p<.05, and accounted for between 1.7% of the variance (Cox & Snell R2=.017) and 4.4% of the variance (Nagelekerke R2=.044). Inclusion in the Early Onset, Not Recent group significantly predicted whether a participant would meet criteria for the high risk group, B=.958, S.E.=.434, p<.05, OR= 2.607. Inclusion in the Early Onset, Recent group was also a significant predictor, B=1.166, S.E.=.437, p<.01, OR = 3.208. Inclusion in the Later Onset, Recent group did not significantly predict whether a participant would meet criteria for the high-risk group.

Discussion

The present study supplements current knowledge on the pathogenesis of BPD though assessment of differences in developmentally salient borderline features in maltreated children, a group at known risk for the development of BPD in adulthood. Our analyses revealed increased rates of borderline features in the maltreated sample relative to an age and socioeconomic status matched comparison group, providing evidence that maltreatment confers considerable risk for development of age-salient borderline personality features in middle childhood. Maltreated children also reported significantly higher levels of all of the borderline features (e.g., affective instability, identity problems, negative relationships and self harm) measured in this examination. Thus, no single borderline trait is producing the differences seen between maltreated and comparison groups. It would seem that all featural facets of BPD may be impacted by maltreatment. Further, maltreated children were over twice as likely to have high scores on all four BPD subscales and thus be considered at high risk for later development of the disorder. These findings are consistent with the extensive literature associating child maltreatment with BPD in adulthood (Battle et al., 2004; ; Johnson et al., 2000).

The distinctive forms of maltreatment were differentially related to borderline feature presentation in childhood. Our data showed that physical abuse and physical neglect were associated with increased overall borderline feature scores. However, this relation was not significant for sexual abuse, a finding contrary to previous literature linking sexual abuse and BPD (; Johnson et al., 1999). One possible explanation for this incongruity lies in sample size. Despite a relatively large maltreatment group, our sexual abuse group (n=27) may not have had enough power to exhibit the effects seen in previous literature. This study's young cohort may also explain the discrepancy. Another study by Widom et al. (2009) that examined BPD in substantiated cases of sexual abuse prior to age 11 also found that sexual abuse was not a significant risk factor for BPD diagnoses in adulthood. It may be the case that some children participating in such studies will subsequently be sexually abused in adolescence, producing results more consistent with the adult retrospective literature. More research is needed to clarify the relation between BPD symptoms and possible influences of developmental timing of sexual abuse.

Alternately, the strong associations between sexual abuse and BPD reported by other researchers, primarily via retrospective investigations, may be due to impacts on outcome variables not seen until adolescence or adulthood such as romantic relationships. If this is the case, either 1) the effects of sexual abuse relative to BPD may be latent during the middle-childhood period, or 2) the middle childhood variables influenced by sexual abuse and associated with BPD were not appropriately measured in this study. Although the former explanation may be true, it is somewhat unlikely if our findings are interpreted through a developmental pathways framework that supports continuity in development. The latter explanation would then be one of heterotypic continuity, the idea that an underlying factor may express itself in different forms at different developmental stages. One developmentally salient variable in middle childhood that may be an earlier manifestation of the influence of sexual abuse is relational aggression (). An investigation by Cullerton-Sen et al. (2008) showed sexual abuse as a predictor of relational aggression for girls. Relational aggression is an established correlate of BPD (Werner & Crick, 1999). Accordingly, future research should be conducted to determine if relational aggression in childhood among girls experiencing sexual abuse is associated with later emergence of BPD in adulthood.

The present study also considers the importance of maltreatment chronicity in the development of borderline symptoms through examination of developmental onset and recency of maltreatment. Specifically, findings showed that the number of developmental periods in which maltreatment occurred significantly predicted borderline feature scores. For every additional developmental period in which maltreatment occurred, there was an increase of 1.284. in an individual's likelihood of meeting criteria for the group with 3 subscale scores 1 standard deviation or more above the mean. Although no prior studies have examined the relation between chronicity of maltreatment and patterns of borderline features, this finding is consistent with prior research showing that chronicity of maltreatment is associated with increases in both externalizing and internalizing problems (Jonson-Reid et al., 2012; Manly et al., 2001). Chronic maltreatment has been shown to impact factors key to the successful maintenance of interpersonal relationships such as prosocial behavior (). The decreased prosocial behavior seen in chronically maltreated children may be a precipitant of relational challenges and higher scores on the Negative Relationships subscale, leading to higher overall borderline feature scores in this analysis.

Differences in overall borderline feature scores were also found between the nonmaltreated group and all maltreated onset and recency groups. However, although each onset and recency group evinced significantly higher overall borderline scores than the nonmaltreated group, no significant differences were found between earlier and later maltreatment onset and recency groups. This is consistent with the findings of Jaffee and Maikovich-Fong (2011) that the effects of chronicity on internalizing and externalizing problems are not dependent on the developmental period when maltreatment began. Manly et al. (2001) also found a similar pattern of significant differences between nonmaltreated and maltreated peers yet limited differences between onset and recency subgroups within the maltreated group. The lack of differences between onset and recency groups may be a result of not accounting for severity and density of maltreatment experiences between maltreatment onset and most recent maltreatment event.

Chronicity of maltreatment was found to influence whether a child displayed significantly higher borderline sub-scores than the mean. Our analysis showed that for each additional developmental period in which maltreatment occurred, an individual is 1.28 times more likely to meet criteria for a high-risk group in which scores on three out of four subscales are one standard deviation or more above the mean.

Onset and recency also appeared to exert influence over the development of borderline personality features. Those in the Early Onset, Not Recent group were 2.61 times more likely to meet criteria for the high-risk group, while those in the Early Onset, Recent group were 3.21 times more likely to meet criteria for the high-risk group. However, inclusion in the Later Onset, Recent group was not a predictor for inclusion in the high-risk group. One possible explanation for this finding is that an early childhood free of maltreatment thus allowed individuals to successfully complete stage-salient developmental tasks that later influence the development of borderline personality features and/or serve as protective factors. Indeed, a number of studies on BPD have linked the disorder to one of the primary developmental milestones of early childhood: forming a secure attachment to a caregiver (; Bowlby, 1988; ; ; Fonagy, 2000; Hill et al., 2011). Maltreatment has long been established in the literature as a risk factor for the formation of an insecure attachment (; ), and attachment theory posits that this attachment serves as the foundation for an internal working model of relationships and self-representation which guides our behavior in future relationships (Bowlby, 1969). Moreover, maltreatment and BPD also are both associated with the disorganized attachment classification (; Levy, 2005; ). In the case of disorganized attachment, repeated contradictory cues and inconsistent caregiving patterns lead to a breakdown in distress regulation strategies in infancy and a conceptualization of the attachment relationship and the self that is fragmented and incoherent (; Main & Solomon,1990). Levy (2005) notes that some researchers have argued that disorganized attachment is at the root of BPD itself (Holmes, 2003, 2004; Liotti, 2000). However, recent work by Carlson, Egeland, and Sroufe (2009) examining the relationship between infant attachment and BPD found that the link between disorganized attachment and adult borderline symptoms may be mediated by the disturbance of self-representational processes in middle childhood and early adolescence. Research implicates maltreatment in the disruption of self system processes and the establishment of a sense of self (see for a review).

This analysis produced no differences in overall BPD precursor scores by gender. The absence of differences between males and females on these traits is not unprecedented; a 2005 study of BPD precursors in children aged 6-12 by Rogosch and Cicchetti also produced no differences in trait rates by gender. However, the finding is noteworthy considering the gender disparity in rates; BPD in adulthood is estimated in 3% of females and 2.4% of males (). These slightly contrasting rates may be due to factors associated with measurement of BPD features in children. Perhaps the adaptation of BPD features to reflect age-salient issues has produced criteria that are more gender-neutral. Alternately, the finding may reflect multifinality in outcomes associated with this developmental pathway. Based on similarities in prevalence, symptoms (e.g., impulsivity) and outcomes (e.g., increased risk for suicide), Paris (1997) has suggested that BPD and antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) may share a common etiology. Beauchaine and colleagues (2009) extended this theory, positing that sex functions as a moderator in the expression of BPD and ASPD as produced through genetic predispositions to impulsivity and environmental influences. Conceivably, findings from this study may reflect individuals who have begun on this maladaptive pathway as a function of maltreatment but have not yet had the behavioral expression moderated by sex. If this is the case, then our findings would suggest that gender does not discriminate BPD from ASPD until some time in adolescence. However, should the two disorders share an underlying etiology, the precipitants of multifinality in BPD and ASPD remain unclear. Future research is needed to test possible mechanisms of influence, including biological or hormonal changes associated with puberty, gender differences in peer relationships, and sociocultural differences in gender roles.

Certain limitations present in this study should be noted. First, although we examined number of subtypes of maltreatment, these analyses do not take into consideration the severity of each maltreatment experience. Examination of chronicity in conjunction with information on the severity of maltreatment may provide a clearer perspective on the impact of maltreatment onset and recency variables in particular. Additionally, this investigation did not include other developmental systems such as attentional networks and emotion regulation systems that are likely influential in production of BPD features (; Posner et al., 2002; ; ). Although beyond the scope of this investigation, individual borderline features and environmental considerations such as maltreatment should be measured in concert with other systems to fully understand the developmental phenomena being examined. This multiple levels of analysis approach will provide a more comprehensive understanding of maladaptive personality processes (; ). Further study of these networks within the sample and the high BPD features group in particular will enable us to better elucidate the processes underlying these behavioral outcomes. Finally, researchers have called for investigations on continuity in underlying personality organization (). As such, longitudinal research on the topic is essential to determine whether individuals high in borderline precursor features at this time point will go on to develop clinical BPD in adulthood.

When examined in conjunction with studies linking childhood trauma and BPD diagnosis in adulthood, results encourage consideration and further review of a multi-level developmental pathway model in which maltreatment may play a role. Should individuals from our identified high-risk group go on to develop the full disorder in adulthood, the BPFS-C may prove an extremely valuable clinical tool for identification of individuals at high risk for BPD and thus candidates for preventative interventions. As BPD is notoriously challenging to effectively treat once canalized (Linehan, 1993; ) early intervention becomes of paramount importance. Prospective longitudinal studies may better clarify the relation among BPD features, chronicity, sexual abuse and gender seen in this investigation. An empirical examination of Beauchaine et al.'s (2009) theory of BPD/ASPD development beginning in early to middle childhood would be especially informative.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by National Institute of Drug Abuse Grants DA12903 and DA17741, National Institute of Mental Health Grant MH83979, and the Spunk Fund, Inc. (to D.C. and F.A.R.). This manuscript is dedicated to the legacy of Nicki R. Crick, a pioneer in the field of child development and developmental psychopathology, and a dear friend.

References

  • Agrawal HR, Gunderson J, Holmes BM, Lyons-Ruth K. Attachment studies with borderline patients: A review. Harvard Review of Psychiatry. 2004;12:94–104. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders. 4th ed., text revision. Washington DC: Author; 2000. [Google Scholar]
  • Ball JS, Links PS. Borderline personality disorder and childhood trauma: evidence for a causal relationship. Current Psychiatry Reports. 2009;11(1):63–68. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Barnett D, Manly JT, Cicchetti D. Defining child maltreatment: the interface between policy and research. In: Cicchetti, Toth, editors. Child Abuse, Child Development and Social Policy. Norwood, NJ: Ablex; 1993. pp. 7–73. [Google Scholar]
  • Battle CL, Shea MT, Johnson DM, Yen S, Zlotnick C, Zanarini MC, et al. Childhood maltreatment associated with adult personality disorders: Findings from the collaborative longitudinal personality disorders study. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2004;18:193–211. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Beauchaine TP, Klien DN, Crowell SE, Debridge C, Gatzke-Kopp L. Multifinality in the development of personality disorders: A Biology x Sex x Environment interaction model of antisocial and borderline traits. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21:735–770. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Belsky DW, Caspi A, Arseneault L, Bleidorn W, Fonagy P, Goodman M, Moffitt TE. Etiological features of borderline personality related characteristics in a birth cohort of 12-year-old children. Development and Psychopathology. 2012;24(1):251. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bemporad JR, Smith HF, Hanson G, Cicchetti D. Borderline syndromes in childhood: Criteria for diagnosis. The American Journal of Psychiatry. 1982;139(5):596–602. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bolger KE, Patterson CJ, Kupersmidt JB. Peer relationships and self-esteem among children who have been maltreated. Child Development. 1998;69:1171–1197. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bowlby J. Attachment. New York: Basic Books; 1969. [Google Scholar]
  • Bowlby J. A secure base Clinical applications of attachment theory. London: Routledge; 1988. [Google Scholar]
  • Bradley R, Jenei J, Westen D. Etiology of borderline personality disorder: Disentangling the contributions of intercorrelated antecedents. Journal of Nervous and Mental Disease. 2005;193:24–31. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bridge JA, Goldstein TR, Brent DA. Adolescent suicide and suicidal behavior. Journal of Child Psychology & Psychiatry. 2006;47(3-4):372–394. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Bradley R, Westen D. The psychodynamics of borderline personality disorder: A view from developmental psychopathology. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17(4):927. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Carlson EA, Egeland B, Sroufe LA. A prospective investigation of the development of borderline personality symptoms. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21(4):1311–1334. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Carlson V, Cicchetti D, Barnett D, Braunwald K. Disorganized/disoriented attachment relationships in maltreated infants. Developmental Psychology. 1989;25(4):525–531. [Google Scholar]
  • Cicchetti D, Barnett D. Attachment organization in maltreated preschoolers. Development and Psychopathology. 1991;3(4):397–411. [Google Scholar]
  • Cicchetti D, Blender JA. A multiple-levels-of-analysis perspective on resilience: Implications for the developing brain, neural plasticity, and preventive interventions. Annals of the New York Academy of Sciences. 2006;1094:248–258. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Cicchetti D, Crick NR. Precursors and diverse pathways to personality disorder in children and adolescents. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21:683–685. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Cicchetti D, Dawson G. Multiple levels of analysis. Development and Psychopathology. 2002;14:417–420. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Cicchetti D, Manly JT. A personal perspective on conducting research with maltreatment families: Problems and solutions. In: Brody E, Sigel I, editors. Family Research: Volume 2. Families at Risk. Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc; 1990. pp. 87–133. [Google Scholar]
  • Cicchetti D, Rogosch FA, Gunnar MR, Toth SL. The differential impacts of early physical and sexual abuse and internalizing problems on daytime cortisol rhythm in school-aged children. Child Development. 2010;81(1):252–269. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Cicchetti D, Toth SL, Manly JT. Maternal Maltreatment Classification Interview. Rochester, NY: Mt. Hope Family Center: Unpublished measure; 2003. [Google Scholar]
  • Cicchetti D, Valentino K. An ecological transactional perspective on child maltreatment:Failure of the average expectable environment and its influence upon child development. In: Cicchetti D, Cohen DJ, editors. Developmental psychopathology. Risk, disorder, and adaptation. 2nd. Vol. 3. New York: Wiley; 2006. [Google Scholar]
  • Crick NR, Murray-Close D, Woods K. Borderline personality features in childhood: A short-term longitudinal study. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17:1051–1070. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Crowell SE, Beauchaine TP, Linehan MM. A biosocial developmental model of borderline personality: Elaborating and extending Linehan's theory. Psychological Bulletin. 2009;135(3):495–510. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Cullerton-Sen C, Cassidy AR, Murray-Close D, Cicchetti D, Crick NR, Rogosch FA. Childhood maltreatment and the development of relational and physical aggression: The importance of a gender-informed approach. Child Development. 2008;79(6):1736–1751. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • De Clercq B, De Fruyt F, Van Leeuwen K, Mervielde I. The structure of maladaptive personality traits in childhood: A step toward an integrative developmental perspective for DSM-V. Journal of Abnormal Personality. 2006;115:639–657. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Egeland B, Sroufe LA. Developmental sequelae of child maltreatment. New Directions for Child Development. 1981;11:77–92. [Google Scholar]
  • English DJ, Upadhyaya MP, Litrownik AJ, Marshall JM, Runyan DK, Graham JC, Dubowitz H. Maltreatment's wake: The relationship of maltreatment dimensions to child outcomes. Child Abuse and Neglect. 2005;29:597–619. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Fonagy P. Attachment and borderline personality disorder. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association. 2000;48(4):1129–1146. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Geiger TC, Crick NR. A developmental psychopathology perspective on vulnerability to personality disorders. In: Ingram, Price, editors. Vulnerability to Psychopathology: Risk across the lifespan. New York: Guilford Press; pp. 57–99. [Google Scholar]
  • Hill J, Stepp SD, Wan MW, Hope H, Morse JQ, Steele M, Steele H, Pilkonis PA. Attachment, borderline personality, and romantic relationship dysfunction. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2011;25(6):789–805. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Herman JL, Perry JC, Van der Kolk BA. Childhood trauma in borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 1989;146:490–495. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Hesse E, Main M. Disorganized infant, child, and adult attachment: Collapse in behavioral and attentional strategies. Journal of the American Psychoanalytic Association. 2000;48(4):1097–1127. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Holmes J. Borderline personality disorder and the search for meaning: An attachment perspective. Australian and New Zealand Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;37(5):524–531. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Holmes J. Disorganized attachment and borderline personality disorder: A clinical perspective. Attachment & Human Development. 2004;6(2):181–190. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Jaffee SR, Maikovich-Fong AK. Effects of chronic maltreatment and maltreatment timing on children's behavior and cognitive abilities. Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry. 2011;52(2):184–194. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Johnson JG, Bromley E, Bornstein RF, Sneed JR. Adolescent personality disorders. In: Wolfe DA, Mash EJ, editors. Behavioral and emotional disorders in children and adolescents: Nature, assessment, and treatment. New York: Guilford Press; 2006. pp. 463–484. [Google Scholar]
  • Johnson JG, Bromley E, McGeoch PG. The role of childhood experiences in the development of adaptive and maladaptive personality traits. In: Oldham JM, Skodol AE, Bender DS, editors. The American Psychiatric Publishing Textbook of Personality Disorders. Washington, D.C: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2005. pp. 209–223. [Google Scholar]
  • Johnson JG, Cohen P, Brown J, Smailes EM, Bernstien DP. Child maltreatment increases risk for personality disorders across early adulthood. Archives of General Psychiatry. 1999;56:600–606. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Johnson JG, Smailes EM, Cohen P, Brown J, Bernstien DP. Associations between four types of childhood neglect and personality disorder symptoms during adolescence and early adulthood: findings of a community-based longitudinal study. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2000;14(2):171–187. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Jonson-Reid M, Kohl PL, Drake B. Child and adult outcomes of chronic child maltreatment. Pediatrics. 2012;129(5):839–845. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Joyce PR, McKenzie JM, Luty SE, Mulder RT, Carter JD, Sullivan PF, Cloninger CR. Temperament, childhood environment and psychopathology as risk factors for avoidant and borderline personality disorders. Australian and New England Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;37(6):756–764. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lieb K, Zanarini MC, Schmahl C, Linehan MM, Bohus M. Borderline personality disorder. Lancet. 2004;364:453–461. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Linehan MM. Cognitive behavioral treatment of borderline personality disorder. New York: Guilford Press; 1993. [Google Scholar]
  • Lenzenweger MF. Epidemiology of personality disorders. Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2008;31(3):395–403. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lenzenweger MF, Cicchetti D. Toward a developmental psychopathology approach to borderline personality disorder. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17:893–898. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Lenzenweger MF, Willett JB. Does change in terperament predict change in schizoid personality disorder? A methodological framework and illustration from the longitudinal study of personality disorders. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21:1211–1231. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Levy KN. The implications of attachment theory and research for understanding borderline personality disorder. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17(4):959. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Liotti G. Disorganized attachment, models of borderline states and evolutionary psychotherapy. In: Bailey KG, Gilbert P, editors. Genes on the couch: Explorations in evolutionary psychotherapy. New York: Routledge; 2000. pp. 232–256. [Google Scholar]
  • Main M, Solomon J. Procedures for identifying infants as disorganized/disoriented during the Ainsworth Strange Situation. Attachment in the Preschool Years: Theory, research, and intervention. 1990;1:121–160. [Google Scholar]
  • Manly JT. Advances in research definitions of child maltreatment. Child Abuse & Neglect. 2005;29:425–439. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Manly JT, Cicchetti D, Barnett D. The impact of subtype, frequency, severity, and chronicity of child maltreatment on social competence and behavior problems. Development and Psychopathology. 1994;6:121–143. [Google Scholar]
  • Manly JT, Kim JE, Rogosch FA, Cicchetti D. Dimensions of child maltreatment and children's adjustment: Contributions of developmental timing and subtype. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:759–782. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Marshall D, English D. Survival analysis of risk factors for recidivism for child abuse and neglect. Child Maltreatment. 1999;4(4):287–296. [Google Scholar]
  • Morey L. Personality Assessment Inventory. Odessa, FL: Psychological Assessment Resources, Inc; 1991. [Google Scholar]
  • Nakash-Eisikovits O, Dutra L, Westen D. Relationship between attachment patterns and personality pathology in adolescents. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry. 2002;41(9):1111–1123. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Paris J. Antisocial and borderline personality disorders: Two separate diagnoses or two aspects of the same psychopathology? Comprehensive Psychiatry. 1997;38:237–242. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Paris J. Recent advances in the treatment of borderline personality disorder. Canadian Journal of Psychiatry. 2005;50(8):435–441. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Posner MI, Rothbart MK, Vizueta N, Levy KN, Evans DE, Thomas KM, Clarkin JF. Attentional mechanisms of borderline personality disorder. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences. 2002;99(25):16366–16370. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Putnam KM, Silk KR. Emotion dysregulation and the development of borderline personality disorder. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17(4):899. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Yarrow MR, Campbell JD, Burton RV. Recollections of childhood: A study of the retrospective method. Monographs of the Society for Research in Child Development. 1970;35(5):iii–83. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rogosch FA, Cichetti D. Child maltreatment, attention networks, and potential precursors to borderline personality disorder. Development and Psychopathology. 2005;17:1071–1089. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Rogosch FA, Cichetti D, Aber JL. The role of maltreatment in early deviations in cognitive and affective processing abilities and later peer relationship problems. Development and Psychopathology. 1995;7:591–609. [Google Scholar]
  • Sedlak A, Mettenburg J, Basena M, Petta I, McPherson K, Greene A, Li S. Fourth National Incidence Study of Child Abuse and Neglect (NIS-4):Report to Congress. Washington D.C: U.S Department of Health and Human Services; 2010. [Google Scholar]
  • Shields A, Cicchetti D. Emotion regulation among school age children: The development and validation of a new criterion Q-sort scale. Development and Psychopathology. 1997;33:906–916. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Shiner RL. The development of personality disorders: Perspectives from normal personality development in childhood and adolescence. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21:715–734. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Skodol AE, Gunderson JG, Pfohl B, Eidiger TA, Livesley WJ, Siever LJ. The borderline diagnosis: Part 1. Psychopathology, comorbidity and personality structure. Biological Psychiatry. 2002;51:936–950. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Sroufe LA. The coherence of individual development: Early care, attachment, and subsequent developmental issues. American Psychologist. 1979;34(10):834. [Google Scholar]
  • Swartz M, Blazer D, George L, Winfield L. Estimating the prevalence of borderline personality disorder in the community. Journal of Personality Disorders. 1984;4:257–272. [Google Scholar]
  • Thornberry TP, Ireland TO, Smith CA. The importance of timing: The varying impact of childhood and adolescent maltreatment on multiple problem outcomes. Development and Psychopathology. 2001;13:957–979. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Tomko RL, Trull TJ, Wood PK, Sher KJ. Characteristics of borderline personality disorder in a community sample: Comorbidity, treatment utilization, and general functioning. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2013;27:1–17. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Toth SL, Cicchetti D, MacFie J, Maughan A, Vanmeenen K. Narrative representations of caregivers and self in maltreated pre-schoolers. Attachment & Human Development. 2000;2(3):271–305. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Trull TJ, Stepp SD, Durett CA. Research on borderline personality disorder: an update. Current Opinion in Psychiatry. 2003;16:77–82. [Google Scholar]
  • Weaver T, Clum G. Early family environments and experiences associated with borderline personality disorder. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1993;61:1068–1075. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Widiger TA, De Clercq B, De Fruyt F. Childhood antecedents of personality disorder: an alternate perspective. Development and Psychopathology. 2009;21:771–791. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Widiger TA, Frances AJ. Epidemiology, diagnosis, and comorbidity of borderline personality disorder. In: Tasman A, Hales RE, editors. Review of Psychiatry. Vol. 8. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Press; 1989. pp. 8–24. [Google Scholar]
  • Widom CS, Czaja SJ, Paris J. A prospective investigation of borderline personality disorder in abused and neglected children followed up into adulthood. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2009;23:433–446. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Widom CS, Raphael K, DuMont K. The case for prospective longitudinal studies in child maltreatment research: Commentary on Dube, Williamson, Thompson, Felitti, & Anda (2004) Child Abuse & Neglect. 2004;28:715–722. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Zanarini MC. Childhood experiences associated with the development of borderline personality disorder. Psychiatric Clinics of North America. 2000;23:89–101. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Hennen J, Silk KR. The longitudinal course of borderline psychopathology: A six-year prospective follow-up of the phenomenology of borderline personality disorder. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2003;160:274–283. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Reich DB, Fitzmaurice G. Attainment and stability of sustained symptomatic remission and recovery among patients with borderline personality disorder and axis II comparison subjects: A 16 year prospective follow-up study. American Journal of Psychiatry. 2012;169(5):476, 483. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Zanarini MC, Frankenburg FR, Ridolfi ME, Jager-Hyman S, Hennen J, Gunderson JG. Reported childhood onset of self-mutilation among borderline patients. Journal of Personality Disorders. 2006;20:9–15. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
Borderline Personality Features in Childhood: The Role of Subtype, Developmental Timing and Chronicity of Child Maltreatment (2024)

FAQs

What are the four subtypes of borderline personality disorder? ›

There are four widely accepted types of borderline personality disorder (BPD): discouraged, impulsive, petulant, and self-destructive BPD. You can suffer more than one kind of BPD simultaneously or at different stages in your life. Similarly, it is also possible for your condition not to fit any of these types of BPD.

What kind of childhood trauma causes borderline personality disorder? ›

Stressful or traumatic life events

Family difficulties or instability, such as living with a parent or carer who experienced an addiction. Sexual, physical or emotional abuse or neglect.

What are the developmental antecedents of borderline personality disorder? ›

Family environment. A child with neurological vulnerabilities combined with invalidating environment can put the child at risk of developing BPD in adulthood. An invalidating environment may be low family cohesion, low maternal support and high levels of maternal-child discord.

What are the key features of borderline personality disorder BPD? ›

According to DSM-IV, the key features of borderline personality disorder are instability of interpersonal relationships, self-image and affect, combined with marked impulsivity beginning in early adulthood.

What are the two most distinguishing features of borderline personality disorder? ›

Borderline Personality Disorder
  • Efforts to avoid real or perceived abandonment, such as plunging headfirst into relationships—or ending them just as quickly.
  • A pattern of intense and unstable relationships with family, friends, and loved ones.
  • A distorted and unstable self-image or sense of self.

What are the three C's of borderline personality disorder? ›

However, it is important that you do not make ultimatums that you cannot carry out and it is vital that you are not tolerant of ongoing abusive behaviour. Remember the 3 C's: I didn't cause it, I can't control it, I can't cure it. If you are experiencing abuse it is important that you seek extra support.

What does borderline personality disorder look like in childhood? ›

Symptoms of borderline personality disorder in a child

an intense fear of abandonment. frequent feelings of emptiness. an aversion to being alone. emotional volatility and inappropriate anger.

What are the childhood precursors of borderline personality disorder? ›

In summary, temperamental traits in childhood, including relational aggression, impulsivity, low emotional control, and negative affectivity, are robust predictors of early onset of BPD.

What is the hardest personality disorder to deal with? ›

Borderline personality disorder historically has been viewed as challenging to treat.

What are the five subtypes of BPD? ›

In response to this problem, Oldham proposed five types of BPD: affective, impulsive, aggressive, dependent and empty.

What are the childlike aspects of borderline personality disorder? ›

They May Appear Childlike.

Many have had to learn about self-care, relationships, or how to be responsible for themselves from the School of Hard Knocks, and as a result, their identity, their understanding of who they are, and their ability to authentically love and care for themselves has been compromised.

What childhood experience is most often associated with borderline personality disorder? ›

Among other types of trauma, BPD is linked to childhood abuse, including sexual, physical, and emotional abuse. BPD features and diagnoses are also associated with having a parent with mental health problems, including depression and stress, and exposure to high levels of negative emotion from a parent.

What is the biggest trait of borderline personality disorder? ›

Individuals with BPD often experience intense and rapidly shifting emotions, have difficulty regulating their emotions, and engage in impulsive behavior, including recurrent self-harm and suicidality.

What are some rarely known signs for BPD? ›

For example, a person with the condition may:
  • Have food habits that strongly resemble those found in a person with bulimia or anorexia.
  • Experience mood alterations that closely mimic major depression or less severe forms of depression.
  • Experience the potentially extreme manic episodes found in people with bipolar disorder.
Jan 31, 2017

What are the strange behaviors of BPD? ›

Impulsive and dangerous behavior: Episodes of reckless driving, fighting, gambling, substance use, binge eating and/or unsafe sexual activity are common among people with BPD. Repeated self-harm or suicidal behavior: People with BPD may cut, burn or injure themselves (self-harm) or threaten to do so.

What are the 4 domains of BPD? ›

When interviewing patients, different domains of symptoms must be explored. Symptoms in borderline personality disorder occur in 4 domains: affectivity, interpersonal functioning, impulse control and cognitive. The diagnosis requires that at least 5 of 9 specific criteria be met (Box 2).

What are the four types of personality disorders? ›

Antisocial personality disorder (ASPD) Borderline personality disorder (BPD) Histrionic personality disorder. Narcissistic personality disorder.

What are the 4 types of borderline mothers? ›

Christine Lawson describes four subtypes of mothers with BPD: the Waif (helpless), the Hermit (fearful/avoidant), the Queen (controlling), and the Witch (sad*stic).

Top Articles
Latest Posts
Article information

Author: Sen. Emmett Berge

Last Updated:

Views: 6092

Rating: 5 / 5 (60 voted)

Reviews: 83% of readers found this page helpful

Author information

Name: Sen. Emmett Berge

Birthday: 1993-06-17

Address: 787 Elvis Divide, Port Brice, OH 24507-6802

Phone: +9779049645255

Job: Senior Healthcare Specialist

Hobby: Cycling, Model building, Kitesurfing, Origami, Lapidary, Dance, Basketball

Introduction: My name is Sen. Emmett Berge, I am a funny, vast, charming, courageous, enthusiastic, jolly, famous person who loves writing and wants to share my knowledge and understanding with you.